Single trial linear regression analysis with the LIMO dataset

Here we explore the structure of the data contained in the LIMO dataset. This example replicates and extends some of the main analysis and tools integrated in LIMO MEEG, a MATLAB toolbox originally designed to interface with EEGLAB.

In summary, the example:

  • Fetches epoched data files for a single subject of the LIMO dataset 1. If the LIMO files are not found on disk, the fetcher mne.datasets.limo.load_data() will automatically download the files from a remote repository.

  • During import, information about the data (i.e., sampling rate, number of epochs per condition, number and name of EEG channels per subject, etc.) is extracted from the LIMO .mat files stored on disk and added to the epochs structure as metadata.

  • Fits linear models on the single subject’s data and visualizes inferential measures to evaluate the significance of the estimated effects.

# Authors: Jose C. Garcia Alanis <alanis.jcg@gmail.com>
#
# License: BSD (3-clause)

import numpy as np
import matplotlib.pyplot as plt

from mne.datasets.limo import load_data
from mne.stats import linear_regression
from mne.viz import plot_events, plot_compare_evokeds
from mne import combine_evoked


print(__doc__)

# subject to use
subj = 1

About the data

In the original LIMO experiment (see 2), participants performed a two-alternative forced choice task, discriminating between two face stimuli. The same two faces were used during the whole experiment, with varying levels of noise added, making the faces more or less discernible to the observer (see Fig 1 in 3 for a similar approach).

The presented faces varied across a noise-signal (or phase-coherence) continuum spanning from 0 to 85% in increasing steps of 5%. In other words, faces with high phase-coherence (e.g., 85%) were easy to identify, while faces with low phase-coherence (e.g., 5%) were hard to identify and by extension very hard to discriminate.

Load the data

We’ll begin by loading the data from subject 1 of the LIMO dataset.

# This step can take a little while if you're loading the data for the
# first time.
limo_epochs = load_data(subject=subj)

Out:

Adding metadata with 2 columns
Replacing existing metadata with 2 columns
1055 matching events found
No baseline correction applied
0 projection items activated
0 bad epochs dropped

Note that the result of the loading process is an mne.EpochsArray containing the data ready to interface with MNE-Python.

print(limo_epochs)

Out:

<EpochsArray |  1055 events (all good), -0.299944 - 0.499907 sec, baseline off, ~213.7 MB, data loaded, with metadata,
 'Face/A': 524
 'Face/B': 531>

Visualize events

We can visualise the distribution of the face events contained in the limo_epochs structure. Events should appear clearly grouped, as the epochs are ordered by condition.

fig = plot_events(limo_epochs.events, event_id=limo_epochs.event_id)
fig.suptitle("Distribution of events in LIMO epochs")
Distribution of events in LIMO epochs

As it can be seen above, conditions are coded as Face/A and Face/B. Information about the phase-coherence of the presented faces is stored in the epochs metadata. These information can be easily accessed by calling limo_epochs.metadata. As shown below, the epochs metadata also contains information about the presented faces for convenience.

Out:

  face  phase-coherence
0    A         0.491210
1    A        -1.235572
2    A        -1.043707
3    A         1.642398
4    A        -0.659978

Now let’s take a closer look at the information in the epochs metadata.

# We want include all columns in the summary table
epochs_summary = limo_epochs.metadata.describe(include='all').round(3)
print(epochs_summary)

Out:

        face  phase-coherence
count   1055         1055.000
unique     2              NaN
top        B              NaN
freq     531              NaN
mean     NaN           -0.000
std      NaN            1.000
min      NaN           -1.619
25%      NaN           -0.852
50%      NaN           -0.084
75%      NaN            0.875
max      NaN            1.642

The first column of the summary table above provides more or less the same information as the print(limo_epochs) command we ran before. There are 1055 faces (i.e., epochs), subdivided in 2 conditions (i.e., Face A and Face B) and, for this particular subject, there are more epochs for the condition Face B.

In addition, we can see in the second column that the values for the phase-coherence variable range from -1.619 to 1.642. This is because the phase-coherence values are provided as a z-scored variable in the LIMO dataset. Note that they have a mean of zero and a standard deviation of 1.

Visualize condition ERPs

Let’s plot the ERPs evoked by Face A and Face B, to see how similar they are.

# only show -250 to 500 ms
ts_args = dict(xlim=(-0.25, 0.5))

# plot evoked response for face A
limo_epochs['Face/A'].average().plot_joint(times=[0.15],
                                           title='Evoked response: Face A',
                                           ts_args=ts_args)
# and face B
limo_epochs['Face/B'].average().plot_joint(times=[0.15],
                                           title='Evoked response: Face B',
                                           ts_args=ts_args)
  • 0.150 s
  • 0.150 s

Out:

No projector specified for this dataset. Please consider the method self.add_proj.
No projector specified for this dataset. Please consider the method self.add_proj.

We can also compute the difference wave contrasting Face A and Face B. Although, looking at the evoked responses above, we shouldn’t expect great differences among these face-stimuli.

# Face A minus Face B
difference_wave = combine_evoked([limo_epochs['Face/A'].average(),
                                  limo_epochs['Face/B'].average()],
                                 weights=[1, -1])

# plot difference wave
difference_wave.plot_joint(times=[0.15], title='Difference Face A - Face B')
0.150 s

Out:

No projector specified for this dataset. Please consider the method self.add_proj.

As expected, no clear pattern appears when contrasting Face A and Face B. However, we could narrow our search a little bit more. Since this is a “visual paradigm” it might be best to look at electrodes located over the occipital lobe, as differences between stimuli (if any) might easier to spot over visual areas.

# Create a dictionary containing the evoked responses
conditions = ["Face/A", "Face/B"]
evokeds = {condition: limo_epochs[condition].average()
           for condition in conditions}

# concentrate analysis an occipital electrodes (e.g. B11)
pick = evokeds["Face/A"].ch_names.index('B11')

# compare evoked responses
plot_compare_evokeds(evokeds, picks=pick, ylim=dict(eeg=(-15, 7.5)))
B11

We do see a difference between Face A and B, but it is pretty small.

Visualize effect of stimulus phase-coherence

Since phase-coherence determined whether a face stimulus could be easily identified, one could expect that faces with high phase-coherence should evoke stronger activation patterns along occipital electrodes.

phase_coh = limo_epochs.metadata['phase-coherence']
# get levels of phase coherence
levels = sorted(phase_coh.unique())
# create labels for levels of phase coherence (i.e., 0 - 85%)
labels = ["{0:.2f}".format(i) for i in np.arange(0., 0.90, 0.05)]

# create dict of evokeds for each level of phase-coherence
evokeds = {label: limo_epochs[phase_coh == level].average()
           for level, label in zip(levels, labels)}

# pick channel to plot
electrodes = ['C22', 'B11']
# create figures
for electrode in electrodes:
    fig, ax = plt.subplots(figsize=(8, 4))
    plot_compare_evokeds(evokeds,
                         axes=ax,
                         ylim=dict(eeg=(-20, 15)),
                         picks=electrode,
                         cmap=("Phase coherence", "magma"))
  • C22
  • B11

As shown above, there are some considerable differences between the activation patterns evoked by stimuli with low vs. high phase-coherence at the chosen electrodes.

Prepare data for linear regression analysis

Before we test the significance of these differences using linear regression, we’ll interpolate missing channels that were dropped during preprocessing of the data. Furthermore, we’ll drop the EOG channels (marked by the “EXG” prefix) present in the data:

limo_epochs.interpolate_bads(reset_bads=True)
limo_epochs.drop_channels(['EXG1', 'EXG2', 'EXG3', 'EXG4'])

Out:

Interpolating bad channels
    Automatic origin fit: head of radius 95.0 mm
Computing interpolation matrix from 113 sensor positions
Interpolating 15 sensors
Number of events 1055
Events Face/A: 524
Face/B: 531
Time range -0.300 – 0.500 sec
Baseline off


Define predictor variables and design matrix

To run the regression analysis, we need to create a design matrix containing information about the variables (i.e., predictors) we want to use for prediction of brain activity patterns. For this purpose, we’ll use the information we have in limo_epochs.metadata: phase-coherence and Face A vs. Face B.

# name of predictors + intercept
predictor_vars = ['face a - face b', 'phase-coherence', 'intercept']

# create design matrix
design = limo_epochs.metadata[['phase-coherence', 'face']].copy()
design['face a - face b'] = np.where(design['face'] == 'A', 1, -1)
design['intercept'] = 1
design = design[predictor_vars]

Now we can set up the linear model to be used in the analysis using MNE-Python’s func:linear_regression function.

Out:

Fitting linear model to epochs, (25728 targets, 3 regressors)
Done

Extract regression coefficients

The results are stored within the object reg, which is a dictionary of evoked objects containing multiple inferential measures for each predictor in the design matrix.

print('predictors are:', list(reg))
print('fields are:', [field for field in getattr(reg['intercept'], '_fields')])

Out:

predictors are: ['face a - face b', 'phase-coherence', 'intercept']
fields are: ['beta', 'stderr', 't_val', 'p_val', 'mlog10_p_val']

Plot model results

Now we can access and plot the results of the linear regression analysis by calling reg['<name of predictor>'].<measure of interest> and using the plot_joint() method just as we would do with any other evoked object. Below we can see a clear effect of phase-coherence, with higher phase-coherence (i.e., better “face visibility”) having a negative effect on the activity measured at occipital electrodes around 200 to 250 ms following stimulus onset.

reg['phase-coherence'].beta.plot_joint(ts_args=ts_args,
                                       title='Effect of Phase-coherence',
                                       times=[0.23])
0.230 s

Out:

No projector specified for this dataset. Please consider the method self.add_proj.

We can also plot the corresponding T values.

# use unit=False and scale=1 to keep values at their original
# scale (i.e., avoid conversion to micro-volt).
ts_args = dict(xlim=(-0.25, 0.5),
               unit=False)
topomap_args = dict(scalings=dict(eeg=1),
                    average=0.05)

fig = reg['phase-coherence'].t_val.plot_joint(ts_args=ts_args,
                                              topomap_args=topomap_args,
                                              times=[0.23])
fig.axes[0].set_ylabel('T-value')
0.230 s

Out:

No projector specified for this dataset. Please consider the method self.add_proj.

Conversely, there appears to be no (or very small) systematic effects when comparing Face A and Face B stimuli. This is largely consistent with the difference wave approach presented above.

ts_args = dict(xlim=(-0.25, 0.5))

reg['face a - face b'].beta.plot_joint(ts_args=ts_args,
                                       title='Effect of Face A vs. Face B',
                                       times=[0.23])
0.230 s

Out:

No projector specified for this dataset. Please consider the method self.add_proj.

References

1

Guillaume A. Rousselet. LIMO EEG dataset. 2016. doi:10.7488/ds/1556.

2

Guillaume A. Rousselet, Carl M. Gaspar, Cyril R. Pernet, Jesse S. Husk, Patrick J. Bennett, and Allison B. Sekuler. Healthy aging delays scalp EEG sensitivity to noise in a face discrimination task. Frontiers in Psychology, 1(19):1–14, 2010. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00019.

3

Guillaume A. Rousselet, Cyril R. Pernet, Patrick J. Bennett, and Allison B. Sekuler. Parametric study of EEG sensitivity to phase noise during face processing. BMC Neuroscience, 9(1):98, 2008. doi:10.1186/1471-2202-9-98.

Total running time of the script: ( 0 minutes 35.224 seconds)

Estimated memory usage: 418 MB

Gallery generated by Sphinx-Gallery